The hardware and bandwidth for this mirror is donated by METANET, the Webhosting and Full Service-Cloud Provider.
If you wish to report a bug, or if you are interested in having us mirror your free-software or open-source project, please feel free to contact us at mirror[@]metanet.ch.

Bayesian Interval Estimation for Repeated-Measures Designs

codecov R-CMD-check

For both the homoscedastic and heteroscedastic cases in one-way within-subject (repeated-measures) designs, this Stan-based R package provides multiple methods to construct the credible intervals for condition means, with each method based on different sets of priors. The emphasis is on the calculation of intervals that remove the between-subjects variability that is a nuisance in within-subject designs, as proposed in Loftus and Masson (1994), the Bayesian analog proposed in Nathoo, Kilshaw, and Masson (2018), and the adaptation presented in Heck (2019).

Installation

Type Source Command
Release CRAN install.packages("rmBayes")
Development GitHub remotes::install_github("zhengxiaoUVic/rmBayes")

R 4.0.1 or later is recommended. Prior to installing the package, you need to configure your R installation to be able to compile C++ code. Follow the link below for your respective operating system for more instructions (version 2.26 or later; Stan Development Team, 2024):

Installation time of the source package is about 11 minutes (Stan models need to be compiled). If you have R version 4.0.1 or later on Mac, Windows, or Ubuntu, you can find the binary packages HERE. Or, directly install the binary package by preferably calling

install.packages("rmBayes", type = "binary")

Statistical Model

When the homogeneity of variance holds, a linear mixed-effects model for the mean response in a one-way within-subject design is

where represents the mean response for the th subject under the th level of the experimental manipulation; is the overall mean, is the th level of the experimental manipulation; , for the means model, is the th condition mean; are the standardized subject-specific random effects; is the number of levels; is the number of subjects. The effects and are both standardized relative to the standard deviation of the error and become dimensionless (Rouder, Morey, Speckman, & Province, 2012).

Method 0

Nathoo et al. (2018) derived a Bayesian within-subject interval by conditioning on maximum likelihood estimates of the subject-specific random effects. An assumption articulated in Method 0 is the Jeffreys prior for the condition means and residual variance , i.e.,

Method 0 constructs the highest-density interval (HDI) as

Note: The sample mean for the th condition is .

The sample mean for the th subject is .

The overall mean is .

The within-group sum-of-squares (SS) is .

The interaction SS is .

refers to a critical value for the -distribution.

library(rmBayes)

str(recall.long)

rmHDI(recall.long, method = 0)

Methods 4-6

Heck (2019) proposed modifying the conditional within-subject Bayesian interval to account for uncertainty and shrinkage in the estimated random effects. He derived a modification by applying the HDI equation in Method 0 for the within-subject Bayesian interval at each iteration of a Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling algorithm and then taking the average interval across posterior samples. Priors used in Method 4 are the Jeffreys prior for the condition means and residual variance, a -prior structure for standardized subject-specific random effects (i.e., ), and independent scaled inverse-chi-square priors with one degree of freedom for the scale hyperparameters of the -priors (i.e., ).

Method 4 constructs the HDI as

rmHDI(recall.long, method = 4, seed = 277)

To assess the robustness of HDI results with respect to the choice of a prior distribution for the standard deviation of the subject-specific random effects in the within-subject case, two additional priors are considered: uniform and half-Cauchy ( or ) for Methods 5 and 6, respectively.

rmHDI(recall.long, method = 5, seed = 277)
rmHDI(recall.long, method = 6, seed = 277)

Methods 1 (Default) and 2-3

Methods 0 and 4-6 arbitrarily assume improper uniform priors for the condition means. In this work, Wei, Nathoo, and Masson (2023) expanded the space of possible priors by appling the HDI equation in Method 0 to derive the newly proposed intervals from MCMC sampling, but assuming default -priors for standardized treatment effects. In other words, Method 1 uses the Jeffreys prior for the overall mean (rather than the condition means), i.e.,

rmHDI(recall.long, seed = 277)

Similar to Methods 5 and 6, two additional priors are considered: uniform and half-Cauchy ( or ) for Methods 2 and 3, respectively, for the standard deviation of the subject-specific random effects in the within-subject case.

rmHDI(recall.long, method = 2, seed = 277)
rmHDI(recall.long, method = 3, seed = 277)

Standard HDI

MCMC sampling of condition means can also be used to obtain the standard HDI, which, unlike Methods 0-6, does not remove the between-subjects variability that is not of interest in within-subject designs. This method assumes the Jeffreys prior for the overall mean and residual variance, a -prior structure for standardized treatment effects, and independent scaled inverse-chi-square priors with one degree of freedom for the scale hyperparameters of the -priors.

rmHDI(recall.long, design = "between", seed = 277)

Random Versus Fixed Effects

When modeling fixed effects, Rouder et al. (2012, p. 363) proposed default priors by projecting a set of main effects into parameters such that , , and ,

where is the identity matrix of size , is the all-ones matrix of size , is an matrix of the eigenvectors of unit length corresponding to the nonzero eigenvalues of , and is the row vector.

rmHDI(recall.long, treat = "fixed", seed = 277)

Heteroscedasticity

When the homogeneity of variance does not hold, the resulting HDI widths for conditions are unequal. Two approaches are currently provided for the heteroscedastic within-subject data: Implementing the approach developed by Nathoo et al. (2018, p. 5);

rmHDI(recall.long, method = 0, var.equal = FALSE)

Or, implementing the heteroscedastic standard HDI method on the subject-centering transformed data (subtracting from the original response the corresponding subject mean minus the overall mean). If a method option other than 0 or 1 is used with var.equal=FALSE, a pooled estimate of variability will be used just as in the homoscedastic case, and a warning message will be returned.

rmHDI(recall.long, var.equal = FALSE)

MCMC Diagnostics

Check the Rhat statistic and effective sample size of MCMC draws.

rmHDI(recall.long, seed = 277, diagnostics = TRUE)$diagnostics

References

Heck, D. W. (2019). Accounting for estimation uncertainty and shrinkage in Bayesian within-subject intervals: A comment on Nathoo, Kilshaw, and Masson (2018). Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 88, 27–31.

Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.

Nathoo, F. S., Kilshaw, R. E., & Masson, M. E. J. (2018). A better (Bayesian) interval estimate for within-subject designs. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 86, 1–9.

Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Speckman, P. L., & Province, J. M. (2012). Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 356–374.

Stan Development Team (2024). RStan: the R interface to Stan. R package version 2.32.5 https://mc-stan.org

Wei, Z., Nathoo, F. S., & Masson, M. E. J. (2023). Investigating the relationship between the Bayes factor and the separation of credible intervals. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30, 1759–1781.

These binaries (installable software) and packages are in development.
They may not be fully stable and should be used with caution. We make no claims about them.